Impact

‘Nudging’: What Works and Why (Not)? Impact
One nudge that worked

‘Nudging’: What Works and Why (Not)?

June 5, 2014 1819

UK organ donor card

One nudge that worked

Earlier this week an impressive cast of academics, policy experts and business leaders gathered in Sydney at the inaugural Behavioural Exchange meeting to talk about “nudges.” (See also: “Drinking For Free at the Behavioral Insights Conference.”)

Made famous by Richard Thaler and Cass Sunstein’s 2008 book, nudges build on almost half a century of work at the intersection of psychology, behavioral economics and policy.

In a nutshell, a nudge is an attempt to make judgements and choices easier – but not in a coercive way.

The pioneers of the approach are the UK Behavioural Insights Team who in conjunction with UK government led by prime minister David Cameron have used nudges to increase the number of organ donors, improve payment rates of fines, and make job-seekers more engaged and involved (among many other things).

Some of these successes have been replicated by the Behavioural Insights Team here in NSW. For example, a study run with the State Debt Recovery Office (SDRO) demonstrated that fine notices which included a prominent “PAY NOW” stamp, and used wording like “you owe” rather than “amount owed” led to significant improvements in payment rates in comparison to a standard letter.

The Conversation logo_AU

This article by Ben Newell originally appeared at The Conversation, a Social Science Space partner site, under the title “’Nudging’ people towards changing behaviour: what works and why (not)?

These success stories are encouraging, but they belie an important question that was perhaps not given enough attention at the meeting. Why do some nudges work and others fail?

The standard argument is that nudges work because they make choices simpler by capitalizing on the “boundedly rational” nature of human decision making. But how, exactly, they work and which aspect of the “choice architecture” is simpler, more engaging, or more influential, can be unclear.

PAY NOW or ‘you owe’

For example was it the “PAY NOW” or the “you owe” that changed payment rates? This might not matter. If the goal is to improve the outcome (that is, encourage people to pay a fine on time), then perhaps understanding the process is not so crucial.

From a psychological perspective, however, it is also important to understand the process or the mechanism. For instance, in the SDRO study, a letter that had the words “ACT NOW” instead of “PAY NOW” was not so successful in changing behavior – why not?

We can speculate but we don’t really know. Knowing why is important not just academically, but also from a practical perspective. If we don’t know why a nudge worked in the first place – and then it stops working (e.g., people return to being tardy fine-payers) – we may not know how to get it working again. Not knowing the why also makes it more difficult to generalize nudges to other contexts.

As many of the speakers at the conference acknowledged, a lot of the successful nudges have not been in place long enough to judge their long term success. Again, sometimes this may not matter – if the nudge is a “set and forget” such as changing defaults to become an organ donor, then nudging people to make the “right” choice once is enough. But nudging repetitive behaviors (for example, energy use in the home) is likely to require repeated reminders to avoid relapses or people becoming habituated to the message.

Replication and the danger of file drawers

Via teleconference, Richard Thaler reminded the audience of the key importance of replicating successful nudges – and also of recording and telling people about “failed” nudges.

If we don’t know why a nudge worked in the first place – and then it stops working – we may not know how to get it working again

Psychology has been through a painful period of self-reflection recently due to highly publicized failures-to-replicate. Part of the problem has been a publication bias whereby experiments that “don’t work” get stuck in the file drawer and no one learns from them.

The field of behavioral insights would do well not to fall foul of a file draw problem and to resist the temptation to over-sell the product too soon. It is worth remembering that much of the pioneering work of Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky – on which behavioral insights are based – focused on situations when people’s reasoning did not “work.” We learn from errors and failures as much as from successes.

The machinery for doing this replication work is readily available. One of the most important messages of the meeting was emphasizing the use of randomized controlled trials and the need for repeated testing and adaptation.

But these sorts of trials are expensive and time consuming and in some sectors there may not be much appetite for replications with larger samples. Once something “works” there might be a temptation, amongst some, to just “run with it.” It is crucial, however, for the continued success of the field that these replications are done (despite the clear practical challenges), and that failures to replicate are reported.

The enthusiasm for and promise of behavioral insights was very evident over the two days of the conference. The future appears bright for practitioners of “BI”, but as UK’s Behavioural Insights director David Halpern noted in his closing remarks, one needs to be cautious and not get swept up in the rhetoric.

Focusing a little more on the “why” and “why not” questions might just provide the kinds of insights necessary to nudge the field forward.The Conversation

***
Ben Newell attended Behavioural Exchange 2014 as a guest of the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation. Ben Newell receives funding from the Australian Research Council.


Ben Newell is an associate professor in the School of Psychology at the University of New South Wales. He is also an associate investigator in the ARC Centre of Excellence in Climate System Science. His research interests include judgment and decision-making, with particular interest in the cognitive mechanisms underlying the decisions we make. He is co-author of the book Straight Choices: The Psychology of Decision Making.

View all posts by Ben Newell

Related Articles

Paper Opening Science to the New Statistics Proves Its Import a Decade Later
Impact
July 2, 2024

Paper Opening Science to the New Statistics Proves Its Import a Decade Later

Read Now
Megan Stevenson on Why Interventions in the Criminal Justice System Don’t Work
Social Science Bites
July 1, 2024

Megan Stevenson on Why Interventions in the Criminal Justice System Don’t Work

Read Now
A Milestone Dataset on the Road to Self-Driving Cars Proves Highly Popular
Impact
June 27, 2024

A Milestone Dataset on the Road to Self-Driving Cars Proves Highly Popular

Read Now
How ‘Dad Jokes’ Help Children Learn How To Handle Embarrassment
Insights
June 14, 2024

How ‘Dad Jokes’ Help Children Learn How To Handle Embarrassment

Read Now
How Social Science Can Hurt Those It Loves

How Social Science Can Hurt Those It Loves

David Canter rues the way psychologists and other social scientists too often emasculate important questions by forcing them into the straitjacket of limited scientific methods.

Read Now
Why Social Science? Because It Can Help Contribute to AI That Benefits Society

Why Social Science? Because It Can Help Contribute to AI That Benefits Society

Social sciences can also inform the design and creation of ethical frameworks and guidelines for AI development and for deployment into systems. Social scientists can contribute expertise: on data quality, equity, and reliability; on how bias manifests in AI algorithms and decision-making processes; on how AI technologies impact marginalized communities and exacerbate existing inequities; and on topics such as fairness, transparency, privacy, and accountability.

Read Now
Digital Scholarly Records are Facing New Risks

Digital Scholarly Records are Facing New Risks

Drawing on a study of Crossref DOI data, Martin Eve finds evidence to suggest that the current standard of digital preservation could fall worryingly short of ensuring persistent accurate record of scholarly works.

Read Now
0 0 votes
Article Rating
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

0 Comments
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments