International Debate

What happened to theory in the 21st century?

January 12, 2011 1527

The last two decades have brought to psychology a renewed attempt to integrate both new forms of cognition (embodied, distributed cognition) as well as integrating the neurosciences into the mainstream of the discipline.  Hence theory in its traditional form continues apace, if by traditional we mean something like the systematic and formal appraisal of research findings, speculative arguments emerging from those findings and some rather loose attempts to predict psychological phenomena based on developments in other research areas.

When looking for the import of the new neuro-cognitive sciences one need look no further than certain philosophers of mind, as well as neurologists who have promised us a brave new world with such tantalizing titles as Neuroscience and Philosophy: Brain, Mind, and Language (Daniel Dennett in 2007) and Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain (Antonio Damasio in 2010).  Like their books on consciousness in the 1990s, however, these commentaries on the current fashions of the day promise more than they offer.

Is theory in the social sciences, and psychology in particular, to be confined to the dictates of the neuro and cognitive sciences?  Hardly.  While post-modernism and its cognates (post-structuralism among them) were retrenching and while the ‘new’ brain sciences threatened to devour what was left of the discipline, psychology’s many ‘others’ continued to produce a steady diet of theory that was neither mainstream neuro-cognitive nor dedicated to a relativist project of linguistic and lingual jouissance.  In our recent 20th anniversary issue of Theory & Psychology published in December 2010 we celebrated these traditions, both critical and positive.

Among these papers are clear calls for a renewed effort in theory construction, a tradition that has survived despite the long positivist and mechanistic traditions of psychology.  For example, Gerd Gigerenzer issued a plea for an end to the toothbrush view of theories – the notion that everyone has their own but wouldn’t be caught dead using someone else’s.  Much of what is called theory in psychology he argues is often merely a surrogate for theory, not an actual theory itself.  These surrogates can consist of ‘circular restatements’ of theories, the ‘one-word explanation’ and the ‘list of dichotomies’ form of theorizing.  He proceeds to give a number of cogent examples in psychology, all of which point to the paucity of theory production and integration.

What is interesting about our special issue, however, is not only the renewed call for theory but also the wide range of possibilities that theorists have posited as possible alternatives.  Obviously not everything can be integrated, some theory must be wrong, some theory will wither on the vine, and others will continue to develop.  But the house of psychology remains sufficiently large to support broad alternative visions of the discipline.

What determines what theory will survive and what will not?  If anything has become clear over the last decades, it is rarely a crucial empirical test or the provision of testable outcomes that turn out to be giant slayers.  These can be important of course in restricted tests but theories are much more complex than that: they thrive in cultural contexts that affect the professional identities of the investigators, they allow the psychologist to promulgate particular views of human nature in opposition to others, they live independent lives in epistemic cultures that thrive under certain social conditions that the theorist may not be fully aware of in the first instance.

Hence theories are often tokens of membership, they indicate which club you belong to or, more importantly, which one you wish to join.  They also propose certain views of human domains that may be ignored in other circles, or may be a route to new understandings and practices.  In that sense, the ‘catholic’ nature of theoretical psychology ought to be championed and continued attempts at unification should be resisted for that very reason.

Related Articles

From the University to the Edu-Factory: Understanding the Crisis of Higher Education
Industry
November 25, 2024

From the University to the Edu-Factory: Understanding the Crisis of Higher Education

Read Now
Deciphering the Mystery of the Working-Class Voter: A View From Britain
Insights
November 14, 2024

Deciphering the Mystery of the Working-Class Voter: A View From Britain

Read Now
Julia Ebner on Violent Extremism
Insights
November 4, 2024

Julia Ebner on Violent Extremism

Read Now
Emerson College Pollsters Explain How Pollsters Do What They Do
International Debate
October 23, 2024

Emerson College Pollsters Explain How Pollsters Do What They Do

Read Now
All Change! 2024 – A Year of Elections: Campaign for Social Science Annual Sage Lecture

All Change! 2024 – A Year of Elections: Campaign for Social Science Annual Sage Lecture

With over 50 countries around the world holding major elections during 2024 it has been a hugely significant year for democracy as […]

Read Now
‘Settler Colonialism’ and the Promised Land

‘Settler Colonialism’ and the Promised Land

The term ‘settler colonialism’ was coined by an Australian historian in the 1960s to describe the occupation of a territory with a […]

Read Now
Webinar: Banned Books Week 2024

Webinar: Banned Books Week 2024

As book bans and academic censorship escalate across the United States, this free hour-long webinar gathers experts to discuss the impact these […]

Read Now
0 0 votes
Article Rating
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

2 Comments
Newest
Oldest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments
cnox

Thank you Mr. Stam for writing this article. As a psychologist I strongly agree with you. As a young scientist I feel that I need to add one point. Being thrown out in the life in between the Master, where one could more or less “pick” a theory and use it and the life as a Ph.D. student often economic factors make us beliefers in one “theoretic religion” or the other. It doesn’t even have to be ones own professor, but rather a current trend in the specific scientific community and the monetary pressure that makes one change ones theory… Read more »