Communication

How Science Communication Can Validate Sexism Communication
It's not a pernicious stereorotype. I read that it's science!

How Science Communication Can Validate Sexism

February 12, 2015 1832

It's not a pernicious stereorotype. I read that it's science!

It’s not a pernicious stereorotype. I read that it’s science!

“Brain Study Confirms Gender Stereotypes,” “Why Men Are Better at Map Reading,” “Women Wired to Multitask,” “Women Crap at Parking” – such were the headlines that hailed the publication of a recent neuroscience study, which claimed to reveal marked differences between the brains of men and women. This study, published in late 2013 in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), applied a brain imaging technique called ‘diffusion tensor imaging’ to model the neural connectivity of the brains of 949 young people. Statistical analysis suggested that males showed greater neural connectivity within each brain hemisphere, while female brains demonstrated greater connectivity across hemispheres. Here, apparently, was scientific proof of the fundamentally dichotomous nature of human gender. A storm of media attention ensued.

In many respects, the PNAS paper is emblematic of a renewed interest in neuroscience research on sexual dimorphism – that is, research examining how men’s and women’s brains differ in structure and function. This greater attention to sex differentiation has some distinctly positive implications: biomedical research has historically taken male bodies as the norm, which has disadvantaged women in respect to medical treatment. However, the way much research on sexual differentiation is conducted and communicated has come under intense criticism from scholars in both the natural and social sciences. Scrutiny of the evidence-base for many claims of sex difference produces plentiful examples of methodological weakness and interpretative bias. In particular, neuroscientific studies of sex difference routinely presume a simplistic gender binary in research design and interpretation; ignore large within-sex variation in favor of emphasizing small differences between the sexes; and privilege determinist biogenetic explanations for brain differences over the equally plausible explanation that plastic brains are shaped by systematically different sociocultural experience.

LSE Impact logo

This article by Cliodhna O’Connor originally appeared on the LSE Impact of Social Sciences blog as “’Brain Study Confirms Gender Stereotypes’: How science communication can fuel modern sexism” and is reposted under the Creative Commons license (CC BY 3.0).

These biases in the scientific conduct of many sex difference studies are compounded when the research enters wider society. All too often, as this research moves through the public sphere it becomes not just descriptive, but prescriptive: statements that ‘men/women are like this’ can quickly mutate into insinuations that ‘men/women should be like this.’ Social psychological research confirms that people are more likely to agree with gender stereotypes and behave in discriminatory ways after encountering biological accounts of gender differences. By framing gender differences as biological inevitabilities, this type of neuro-discourse can sanction and sustain prevailing gender hierarchies.

The recruitment of neuroscience to validate traditional gender roles was vividly illustrated in an analysis that Helene Joffe and I conducted of media responses to the aforementioned PNAS study. Our analysis, which was recently published in the open-access journal PLOS ONE, tracked the journey of the PNAS research from its initial scientific publication, through a university-issued press release, into its reception in the traditional news media, online reader comments and blog entries. The analysis systematically documented how, over the course of mediated communication, traditional gender stereotypes are projected onto scientific information about sexual dimorphism. Although the original scientific article did not report any behavioral data, within it the authors speculated that their neuroimaging results underpinned a host of gender differences in cognition and behavior (for instance, explaining men’s supposedly superior sensorimotor skills and women’s ‘intuition’). The press release and subsequent media coverage went further in explicitly describing the research as a validation of existing gender stereotypes, and linking it to behavior differences never mentioned in the original article (for example, women’s purported affinity for ‘multitasking’ and parenting, and men’s greater powers of rationality). The authority of science was harnessed to portray traditional gender stereotypes as factually true, biologically inevitable and socially legitimate.

Our analysis highlights the role played by science in moving modern-day sexism beyond overtly misogynist remarks to more subtle, superficially benign ways of perpetuating gender stereotypes. In our study, a degree of explicit hostility towards women was evident in the readers’ comments. However, derogatory statements characterized only a small minority (about 5 percent) of comments. Across our data as a whole, the predominant message taken from the research was that it highlighted the unique value of men and women’s distinctive strengths. In both the scientific article and its popular commentary, the purported sex differences were portrayed as producing ‘complementary’ inter-sex relationships, whereby each sex’s talents would compensate for the other’s weaknesses. Sex difference was thereby cast as a positive phenomenon that merited celebration.


For more on this study …

… and analysis, the full PLoS ONE article can be accessed here: O’Connor C, Joffe H (2014) Gender on the Brain: A Case Study of Science Communication in the New Media Environment. PLoS ONE 9(10): e110830. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0110830


However, although this message of ‘equal but different’ explicitly invokes egalitarian principles, it is also consistent with what is known in psychology as ‘benevolent sexism.’ In both the scientific paper and its popular commentary, the way in which skills were distributed between male and female brains fit a pattern of ‘complementary stereotyping,’ in which celebration of women’s performance in low-status domains compensates for their relegation from more socially and materially rewarded dimensions. Men were portrayed as logical, focused and physically competent, while women’s strengths lay in emotional intelligence, social skills and caring. Social psychological research suggests that such complementary stereotypes are particularly effective mechanisms for obscuring gender inequality – much more so than overt misogyny, which is easy to recognize and challenge. By affording scientific validation to complementary gender stereotypes, sex difference research may help to bolster enduring gender inequalities in professional, political and domestic contexts. As we argue in our paper, “the neuroscience of sex difference does not merely reflect, but can actively shape the gender norms of contemporary society”.

Our study also furnished an original insight into the dynamics of science communication in the contemporary media environment. In particular, the analysis suggested that the press release can be a ‘point of no return’ in dictating the direction of media coverage of a scientific study. As the press release was often journalists’ sole source, information that was lost between the scientific article and its press release rarely resurfaced, while topics that were newly introduced in the press release could develop into focal points of media coverage. In the traditional media in particular, dependence on the press release gave rise to highly standardized coverage across media outlets.

However, the analysis also illustrated the importance of new media in diversifying public discussion of science. Blogs and comments allow people to articulate their more personalized, spontaneous responses to scientific messages. This can mean, as in the case of the readers’ comments discussed above, the circulation of pejorative or reactionary statements that would not satisfy the editorial restrictions of more formal media outlets. However, in our study blogs and comments also offered a platform for objecting to the socially conservative interpretations of the research that dominated the traditional press. These online spaces were critical in facilitating more nuanced debate about the social implications of the research, and its potential to perpetuate gender stereotypes and inequalities. The new media bring a vastly increased variety of voices to science communication, which can open up more inclusive and dynamic debates about what emerging scientific findings mean for the local realities of individuals and communities.

Finally, the analysis highlights the limitations of conceptualizing science communication in terms of a ‘pure’ science that is set against a biased media. The scientific paper itself and its university-issued press release contained numerous instances of unwarranted extrapolation from the data collected. It was also notable that some of the more dubious features of media coverage, such as the claim that the data explained women’s greater affinity for parenting, were apparently fuelled by quotes the researchers themselves provided to journalists. Cultural models of gender do not vanish when a citizen puts their scientist hat on: these cultural understandings persist in the presuppositions that guide the selection, pursuit and interpretation of scientific questions. We need to begin cultivating awareness of how gender stereotypes become reified by these constant feedback-loops between the understandings of science and society.


Cliodhna O’Connor is a postdoctoral researcher in social psychology. Her research explores how scientific knowledge travels through wider society, and its implications for social identities​, public attitudes and common-sense beliefs.

View all posts by Cliodhna O'Connor

Related Articles

Let’s Return to Retractions Being Corrective, Not Punitive
Communication
July 15, 2024

Let’s Return to Retractions Being Corrective, Not Punitive

Read Now
Uncovering ‘Sneaked References’ in an Article’s Metadata
Communication
July 11, 2024

Uncovering ‘Sneaked References’ in an Article’s Metadata

Read Now
Fifth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: Do Peacebuilding Practices Exclude Women?
Bookshelf
June 27, 2024

Fifth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: Do Peacebuilding Practices Exclude Women?

Read Now
How ‘Dad Jokes’ Help Children Learn How To Handle Embarrassment
Insights
June 14, 2024

How ‘Dad Jokes’ Help Children Learn How To Handle Embarrassment

Read Now
Fourth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: Do Women Make Better Doctors? 

Fourth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: Do Women Make Better Doctors? 

In this issue of The Evidence newsletter, journalist Josephine Lethbridge examines why women doctors see better outcomes in their patients’ health.

Read Now
Webinar – Trust in Science: Understanding the Trends and Implications for Science Communication

Webinar – Trust in Science: Understanding the Trends and Implications for Science Communication

Recent survey data show declines in trust in science that mirror earlier trends for other institutions, including journalism and government. New research […]

Read Now
Celebrating 20 Years of an Afrocentric Small Scholarly Press

Celebrating 20 Years of an Afrocentric Small Scholarly Press

To mark the Black- and female-owned Universal Write Publications’ 20th anniversary, Sage’s Geane De Lima asked UWP fonder Ayo Sekai some questions about UWP’s past, present and future.

Read Now
0 0 votes
Article Rating
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

3 Comments
Newest
Oldest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments
PRice

At the risk of wading into a quagmire, it’s hard to take this study’s findings seriously, that there are new and significant hardwired differences in human male and female brains in additional to what we already knew, because the authors do not explain all the factors involved in why they found what they did. For example, can we raise kids in our culture along typical gender roles and biases, then at ages 12-14, say that the differences in their brains are solely due to their genders? To do so would be to ignore what is known about epigenetic and environmental… Read more »

prickle

This is one of the most idiotic things I have read in awhile. As a scientists, I am embarrassed that you call yourself a colleague.