Impact

Why Social Science? Leaders Need to Know How to Lead with Evidence

May 26, 2021 3943

Being bad at math can kill people. Even experts who should understand medical science and help us make good health decisions sometimes fail. In December, a doctor—let’s call him Dr. Smith—advised a tweeter’s elderly mom not to get the COVID-19 vaccine because “99% of people fight off COVID, but [the vaccine is] only 93% effective.” This doctor is blatantly wrong. He doesn’t understand the math and is giving life-threatening advice. When even some experts struggle with numbers, it’s easy to see the problem.

2020 thrust a new world of statistics upon us—numbers of cases and deaths, false positive rates, and percentages of open ICU beds. Many of us track these numbers to stay informed and feel secure, but we don’t always understand or use them appropriately. Communicating facts isn’t always enough. But when facts are presented in forms that make sense to us—using the science of science communication—leaders can inform and motivate better choices.

“When facts are presented in forms that make sense to us—using the science of science communication—leaders can inform and motivate better choices.”

It would be a mistake not to get a vaccine because 99% of people fight off COVID while the vaccine’s effectiveness is lower; that would be true even if the vaccine was 50% effective, let alone 93%. Dr. Smith misunderstood how these numbers are related. It’s nonsense to compare the vaccine’s effectiveness to COVID’s survival rate. Assuming it were true that 99% of people survive COVID, it would still be eminently sensible to reduce how many get infected in the first place. If you could prevent half of cases—or better yet, 93% of them—then far fewer people would have to risk death, not to mention other effects of the disease.

Unfortunately, many people don’t understand the logic of these numbers. As a decision psychologist, I was so bothered by Dr. Smith’s faulty reasoning that I asked 737 Americans about what he said. Almost a third (29%) agreed at least somewhat that “if 99% of people fight off COVID, and the vaccine is only 93% effective, I’m better taking my chances rather than getting the vaccine.” People who were worse at math agreed more than the math-proficient. The more someone agreed with this faulty comparison, the less they agreed that enough people getting vaccinated would reduce cases and deaths. This is a comprehension failure.

WhySocialScience logo_
This article by Ellen Peters originally appeared on Why Social Science? as “Because Leaders Need to Know How to Lead with Evidence”

Providing people with COVID-19 statistics should lead to greater understanding and wiser choices. However, many American adults lack basic math skills. About a third of us are innumerate. And pandemic statistics have been particularly
error-prone, even as people try to use them to resolve deep uncertainties and make effective decisions.

As a result, some question whether leaders should provide statistics at all, because numbers might confuse rather than inform or might cause people to double down on incorrect beliefs.

But we know that providing facts often helps. In one experiment, participants read about a drug prescribed to treat high cholesterol. We showed half our participants its side effects and their non-numeric chances (for example, headaches are common). Most mistakenly overestimated the risks.

“Pandemic statistics have been particularly error-prone, even as people try to use them to resolve deep uncertainties and make effective decisions.”

The other half of participants saw the same side effects with numeric likelihoods (seven percent get headaches). They had a more accurate understanding of their chances of experiencing side effects and were more willing to follow doctor recommendations than those who had been given non-numeric information. These findings held whether participants were better or worse at math.

Facts can matter even for politically polarized issues. In another study, Americans underestimated the scientific consensus concerning human-caused climate change. However, when provided statistics, they became more accurate and confident about the consensus. Similar effects emerged across political ideologies.

Leaders should lead with pandemic evidence to reduce misunderstandings and misinterpretations like Dr. Smith’s. They need to be transparent about what is known and not known, including providing statistics indicating where uncertainty lies. Doing so will help correct false facts and misinterpretations while avoiding surprise, regret, and anger when the unexpected occurs. Because people also prefer getting statistics and perceive them as useful, providing statistics will help earn the public’s trust and build healthier behaviors.

Current methods of presenting statistics, however, likely surpass many people’s capacities. Leaders need to do the math for those who cannot or will not do it themselves. Providing simple, easy-to-understand, and appropriate comparisons then will help leaders have greater impact. Visualizing data also can promote comprehension. But designing good data presentations is difficult, and even well-intended communications can lead to worse comprehension.

Leaders should provide appropriate data using the science of how numbers affect us and how to present them. As individuals, we also can ask experts to provide numbers in ways that help us choose better.

Why social science?  Because leaders need to treat the public respectfully as adults who can handle statistics (presented accessibly), understand the gravity of their choices, and take charge of their lives. This is critical today when personal choices—to vaccinate, mask, or quarantine—determine how well we emerge from this pandemic.

Ellen Peters, Ph.D. is a decision psychologist and author of Innumeracy in the Wild: Misunderstanding and Misusing Numbers with Oxford University Press. She is the Philip H. Knight Chair and Director of the Center for Science Communication Research in the University of Oregon’s School of Journalism and Communication. She studies the basic building blocks of human judgment and decision making and their links with effective communication techniques and has published more than 150 peer-reviewed papers on these topics. She is former President of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making and a fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, Association for Psychological Science, the American Psychological Association, and the Society for Experimental Social Psychology. She also works with federal agencies to advance decision and communication sciences in health and health policy, including having been Chair of FDA’s Risk Communication Advisory Committee and member of the NAS’s Science of Science Communication committee. She has been awarded the Jane Beattie Scientific Recognition Award and an NIH Group Merit Award. Finally, she has received extensive funding from the National Science Foundation and National Institutes of Health.

View all posts by Ellen Peters

Related Articles

Canada’s Storytellers Challenge Seeks Compelling Narratives About Student Research
Communication
November 21, 2024

Canada’s Storytellers Challenge Seeks Compelling Narratives About Student Research

Read Now
Tom Burns, 1959-2024: A Pioneer in Learning Development 
Impact
November 5, 2024

Tom Burns, 1959-2024: A Pioneer in Learning Development 

Read Now
Ninth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: Tackling the Gender Pay Gap 
Communication
October 31, 2024

Ninth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: Tackling the Gender Pay Gap 

Read Now
The Conversation Podcast Series Examines Class in British Politics
Communication
October 25, 2024

The Conversation Podcast Series Examines Class in British Politics

Read Now
Emerson College Pollsters Explain How Pollsters Do What They Do

Emerson College Pollsters Explain How Pollsters Do What They Do

As the U.S. presidential election approaches, news reports and social media feeds are increasingly filled with data from public opinion polls. How […]

Read Now
Diving Into OSTP’s ‘Blueprint’ for Using Social and Behavioral Science in Policy

Diving Into OSTP’s ‘Blueprint’ for Using Social and Behavioral Science in Policy

Just in time for this past summer’s reading list, in May 2024 the White House Office of Science and Technology Policy (technically, […]

Read Now
Eighth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: How Sexist Abuse Undermines Political Representation 

Eighth Edition of ‘The Evidence’: How Sexist Abuse Undermines Political Representation 

In this month’s issue of The Evidence newsletter, Josephine Lethbridge explores rising levels of abuse directed towards women in politics, spotlighting research […]

Read Now
0 0 votes
Article Rating
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

1 Comment
Newest
Oldest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments
Russell John Foote

Very commendable article, Professor Peters. Let us begin to propose and vigorously champion solutions even if it means establishing an international organization to do so. Social Scientists worldwide should begin to establish regional research associations and university based research organizations with the objective of investigating real world social problems that governments clearly need help p to reduce and resolve. Financial allocations to deal with such problems are necessary but clearly insufficient. The purpose of the research thrust must not be the pursuit of self aggrandizement and citations but all such research must generate and be the basis of an action… Read more »